Showing Results for
- Academic Journals (53)
Search Results
- 53
Academic Journals
- 53
-
From:Nature (Vol. 465, Issue 7297) Peer-ReviewedThe sweet smell of the past. Author(s): Misha Angrist 1 Author Affiliations: (1) Misha Angrist is at the Duke Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy. His book Here is a Human Being: At the Dawn of Personal...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 8, Issue 11) Peer-ReviewedBased on animal studies and some indirect clinical evidence, dopamine has been suggested to have anti-nociceptive effects. Here, we investigated directly the effects of increased and decreased availability of...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 7, Issue 6) Peer-ReviewedAuthor(s): Ludger Elling 1 , * , Harald Schupp 2 , Janine Bayer 3 , Ann-Kathrin Bröckelmann 1 , Christian Steinberg 1 , Christian Dobel 1 , Markus Junghofer 1 Introduction It has been suggested that, under acute...
-
From:Current Psychology (Vol. 32, Issue 4) Peer-ReviewedThe human visual system employs a sophisticated set of strategies for scanning the environment and directing attention to stimuli that can be expected given the context and a person's past experience. Although these...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 9, Issue 3) Peer-ReviewedAuthor(s): Seon-Koo Lee 1,2, Ji Won Chun 1, Jung Suk Lee 1,3, Hae-Jeong Park 4, Young-Chul Jung 1,5, Jeong-Ho Seok 1,5, Jae-Jin Kim 1,4,5,* Introduction Salience refers to the state or quality of an item that...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 7, Issue 5) Peer-ReviewedHuman infants, like immature members of any species, must be highly selective in sampling information from their environment to learn efficiently. Failure to be selective would waste precious computational resources on...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 7, Issue 5) Peer-ReviewedA number of studies have shown that emotionally arousing stimuli are preferentially processed in the human brain. Whether or not this preference persists under increased perceptual load associated with a task at hand...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 12, Issue 5) Peer-ReviewedReinforcement learning tasks are often used to assess participants' tendency to learn more from the positive or more from the negative consequences of one's action. However, this assessment often requires comparison in...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 6, Issue 1) Peer-ReviewedThe notion of a saliency-based processing architecture [1] underlying human vision is central to a number of current theories of visual selective attention [e.g., 2]. On this view, focal-attention is guided by an...
-
From:Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research (Vol. 60, Issue 5) Peer-ReviewedPurpose: Language comprehension in people with aphasia (PWA) is frequently evaluated using multiple-choice displays: PWA are asked to choose the image that best corresponds to the verbal stimulus in a display. When a...
-
From:American Journal of Psychology (Vol. 124, Issue 4) Peer-ReviewedSignal salience was manipulated using configural and object displays to examine their effects on the performance, workload, and stress of vigilance. Improving performance and reducing the workload and stress of...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 8, Issue 1) Peer-ReviewedHuman overt attention under natural conditions is guided by stimulus features as well as by higher cognitive components, such as task and emotional context. In contrast to the considerable progress regarding the former,...
-
From:International Archives of Medicine (Vol. 4) Peer-ReviewedBackground In nature, sensory stimuli are organized in heterogeneous combinations. Salient items from these combinations 'stand-out' from their surroundings and determine what and how we learn. Yet, the relationship...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 9, Issue 4) Peer-ReviewedVisual memory has been demonstrated to play a role in both visual search and attentional prioritization in natural scenes. However, it has been studied predominantly in experimental paradigms using multiple...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 5, Issue 9) Peer-ReviewedBackground Selective visual attention is the process by which the visual system enhances behaviorally relevant stimuli and filters out others. Visual attention is thought to operate through a cortical mechanism known...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 7, Issue 11) Peer-ReviewedAuthor(s): Mareike Finke 1 , 2 , Carles Escera 1 , 2 , Francisco Barceló 2 , 3 , * Introduction The brain's ability to represent, maintain and update contextual ( task-set ) information enables us to alternate...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 12, Issue 12) Peer-ReviewedFacial self-resemblance has been associated with positive emotional evaluations, but this effect may be biased by self-face familiarity. Here we report two experiments utilizing startle modulation to investigate how the...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 9, Issue 3) Peer-ReviewedAuthor(s): Gérard Derosière 1,2,*, Sami Dalhoumi 2,3, Stéphane Perrey 1, Gérard Dray 3, Tomas Ward 2 Introduction Attention to a cognitively demanding task cannot be maintained at a high level indefinitely. During...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 9, Issue 7) Peer-ReviewedThe sleeping brain retains some residual information processing capacity. Although direct evidence is scarce, a substantial literature suggests the phase of slow oscillations during deep sleep to be an important...
-
From:PLoS ONE (Vol. 6, Issue 6) Peer-ReviewedOne of the central questions in cognitive neuroscience is the precise neural representation, or brain pattern, associated with a semantic category. In this study, we explored the influence of audiovisual stimuli on the...